Emotional face expression recognition in problematic Internet use … – Nature.com

Posted under Cibercommunity, Technology On By James Steward

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.
Advertisement
Carousel with three slides shown at a time. Use the Previous and Next buttons to navigate three slides at a time, or the slide dot buttons at the end to jump three slides at a time.
10 August 2021
Hayley Darke, Suresh Sundram, … Olivia Carter
31 May 2021
Vanesa Perez, Ruth Garrido-Chaves, … Alicia Salvador
15 November 2022
Qijie Kuang, Sumiao Zhou, … Shenglin She
03 March 2020
Delhii Hoid, Dong-ni Pan, … Xuebing Li
13 September 2020
Cathy Davies, Robin Wilson, … Sagnik Bhattacharyya
30 April 2019
Anya K. Bershad, Leah M. Mayo, … Harriet de Wit
10 February 2020
Frederick S. Barrett, Manoj K. Doss, … Roland R. Griffiths
21 April 2022
Anne Schienle, Isabella Unger & Daniela Schwab
21 October 2020
Sandra Tamm, Johanna Schwarz, … Gustav Nilsonne
Scientific Reports volume 13, Article number: 354 (2023)
775 Accesses
2 Altmetric
Metrics details
Growing literature indicates that problematic Internet use (PIU) and excessive smartphone use (ESU) are associated with breakdown of functional brain networks. The effects of PIU&ESU on emotional face expression (EFE) recognition are not well understood, however behavioural investigations and fMRI studies of different addiction forms indicated the impairment of this function. The Facial Emotion Recognition Paradigm was used to probe cortico-limbic responses during EFE recognition. Combined fMRI and psychophysiological analysis were implemented to measure EFE-related functional brain changes in PIU&ESU. Self-reported questionnaires were used to assess PIU&ESU. Positive associations were found between the extent of PIU&ESU and functional connections related to emotional cognitive control and social brain networks. Our findings highlight the involvement of social functioning, especially EFE recognition in PIU&ESU. Therefore, we emphasize that besides the brain’s executive and reward systems, the social brain network might be the next candidate to be involved in the pathogenesis of PIU&ESU.
Since Internet is an integrated part of our personal and professional lives, investigation of problematic Internet use (PIU) has proliferated in scientific community. PIU is generally defined as a behavioural addiction1,2, that shares characteristics with substance and other behavioural addiction forms. PIU includes uncontrolled Internet use, obsessive thinking about the Internet and neglecting every-day and social life3. PIU could lead to negative consequences in social interactions4, in daily life functions5, and also in professional performance6. Currently it is estimated that 6% of the world’s population is affected by PIU7, suggesting that PIU is considered as a new and relatively fast growing mental health concern.
To understand the neural basis of PIU and other more specific Internet-related addiction forms such as excessive smartphone use (ESU), increased number of magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) investigations have been performed. These studies found structural alterations in the brain reward system8,9 and gray matter alterations in areas involved in decision making processes10. Besides structural changes, system-level functional alterations have also been revealed related to inhibitory control network in subjects with PIU11.
In the conditions of PIU and ESU (hereafter PIU&ESU), social cognitive functions may also be altered12. Social cognition refers to specific cognitive processes which help to decode primer social signals such as emotional facial expressions (EFEs)13. Emotional expressions provide necessary information about mental states, therefore the accurate perception of facial expressions is indispensable for successful social functioning14,15. In functional cooperation with several cortical and subcortical structures16,17,18,19,20 human amygdala plays a crucial role in facial expression recognition21. Amygdala and prefrontal brain structures such as the frontal pole (FP), the dorso-lateral prefrontal cortex and the superior frontal gyrus (SFG) establish an important functional cooperation within the emotional cognitive control network in the regulation and recognition of emotion via frontal regions’ cognitive control functions16,22,23. The alterations of these FCs are related to emotional disturbances and the pathogenesis of different addictive disorders24,25. Via their social cognitive functions, the anterior (ACG) and posterior cingulate gyrus (PCG)17, the middle temporal gyrus (MTG)18 and the supramarginal gyrus (SMG)26 are also involved in facial expression recognition through their functional cooperation with the amygdala. Reduced recognition of EFEs has a negative effect on social cognitive processes in several forms of substance addictions27. Subjects with alcohol use disorder28 show functional alterations in the fronto-limbic circuity29, cocaine abusers have reduced task-based amygdala connectivity with the ACG during facial emotion recognition30 while methamphetamine addicts tend to have social cognitive deficits, which is underlined by increased task related activity in the ACG31.
Since PIU and substance disorders share several similarities, it is essential to investigate social functioning, especially the facial emotion recognition in PIU&ESU. Social cognitive deficits are even more likely to occur in PIU, due to the extensive social life and communication in online space. Poorer emotion recognition performance was found among social network site (SNS) addicts relative to controls32, furthermore, emotion recognition accuracy in subjects with internet gaming disorder (IGD) is an independent predictor of the extent of the addiction33.
The psychological and neural alterations of EFE recognition is not well understood in PIU&ESU since only a small number of researchers are engaged in the topic34. Chun et al.35 found that subjects with ESU show extensive deactivation in emotion-processing brain regions during facial emotion processing, which may be induced by a failure on cognitive control. Besides this, Cheng and his co-workers explored modified resting-state functional connections between the amygdala and brain regions related to emotional functioning (ACG, precuneus, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) in subjects with PIU34. Based on these neuroimaging observations, it is highly needed to investigate amygdala’s functional connectivity (FC) during EFE recognition in PIU&ESU. To investigate these EFE recognition related FCs, the Facial Emotion Recognition Paradigm (FERP)36 is the most frequently used paradigm.
Therefore, in this study our aim was to investigate the effects of PIU and ESU on task-induced functional connections in young adults using fMRI during the FERP. Psychophysiological interaction analysis (PPI) was implemented to get a comprehensive view about the neural background. Since previous studies used only resting-state fMRI approaches to investigate functional connectivity changes, our primary aim was to use a task- and voxel-based method (PPI) to get a comprehensive view about the possible effect of PIU&ESU. Our secondary aim was to increase the sample size since previous studies reported small-to-medium sample sizes (n < 30 in all studies).
According to the previous behavioural, resting-state and task-based fMRI studies, we assume that there are positive associations between bilateral amygdala-based functional connections involved in EFE recognition and the extent of PIU and ESU.
All subjects were recruited through the Internet. A total of 683 adults participated in the online survey on problematic Internet use. After the inclusion procedure 65 Caucasian university students (32 males, mean age = 23.5, SD = 2.91; 33 females, mean age = 21.5, SD = 2), aged between 18 and 30 (mean age = 22.5, SD = 2.68) years were randomly chosen to take part in the study. During the selection process, subjects who reported any potential MRI safety risk factors, psychiatric or neurological symptoms were excluded. The handedness was assessed with the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory37. According to the scores, all participants had right-hand dominance (median, min–max; 80, 30–100). Depression was measured with the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) and only participants scoring less than eighteen were included in the study38. According to self-reported questions subjects spend on average 3.38 (SD = 1.75) hours online per day. For a more detailed description of the selection process see Fig. 1.
Subjects were paid a small fee for their participation in the study. This research was approved by the Regional Research Ethics Committee (7476—PTE 2018). All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments. All subjects signed a written informed consent in the study.
Selection of the final sample. A total of 683 participants completed the online survey, 25 of them reported smoking habits. They were excluded from the selection procedure. All candidates met the expected age range (18–30 years). From the remaining 658 participants 36 females and 36 males were chosen randomly with no MRI safety risk factors. Seven of the 72 subjects scored more than the recommended cutoff score of BDI, therefore they were excluded. Finally, 65 participants took part in the study.
Without well-established diagnostic criteria, it is highly recommended to measure PIU with a multidimensional questionnaire without cutoff points39. Therefore, we did not form control and problematic user groups based on the severity of PIU, rather used it as a continuous variable.
The extent of PIU was measured with the Hungarian version of the PIUQ3,40. PIUQ is a self-reported inventory with good validity and reliability characteristics. The questionnaire contains 18 items, for each item scored on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (always). PIUQ consists of three subscales which are obsession, neglect, and control disorder. Each subscale contains six items. The obsession scale measures the extent of the obsessive thinking of the Internet (e.g. fantasizing and daydreaming) and the withdrawal symptoms induced by the lack of the online state (e.g. depression, anxiety). Neglect refers to negation of daily activities and social life caused by excessive Internet use (e.g., reduced sleep time & essential needs intake). Control disorder subscale assesses the disability of controlling time spent on the Internet. The total score of PIUQ was computed by summing the scores of all three subscales. The overall internal reliability of the PIUQ is good (Cronbach alpha is 0.87)3.
We used the Hungarian version of the SABAS to measure excessive smartphone use41. SABAS is a unidimensional self-reported inventory, with good internal consistency (Cronbach alpha = 0.82) and validity, comprises six items41. These items refer to the six core criteria of the addiction (salience, mood, modification, tolerance, withdrawal conflict and relapse)42. All the items are rated using a 6-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree). The total score of SABAS was computed by summing all six items. Higher score in SABAS indicates a greater risk of developing addiction to smartphone use.
Face photographs used in the FERP were selected from the FACES database43 which is a validated set of faces displaying different emotional states. Photographs depicted young adults (mean age = 24.2, SD = 3.4, age range = 19–31 years) on bright background, all wearing identical standard grey T-shirts without jewellery, glasses, make-up, or other eye-catching items. During their research Ebner and her colleagues found that the age of the face plays a crucial role in facial expression identification e.g., younger people identify young people’s facial expressions more precisely than older and middle-aged43. Therefore, we used images depicted young adults from the database due to our participants age range.
We used presentation software (Neurobehavioral System, Inc., Berkley, CA, USA) to run the paradigm during the fMRI measurement. The visual stimuli were presented on an MRI-compatible display system (Cambridge Research Systems Ltd, BoldScreen 24″, Rochester, UK) by means of a mirror attached to the head coil. All responses from the subject were collected via MR-compatible response buttons (ResponseGrip, NordicNeuroLab AS, Bergen, Norway).
The Facial Emotion Recognition Paradigm (FERP) developed by Hariri and colleagues36 is a well-validated and effective task to probe corticolimbic responses during the identification of emotional facial expressions. According to previous studies the original FERP and its variants have been shown to reliably and robustly engage the amygdala and strongly activates corticolimbic networks involved in emotion generation and regulation36,44,45.
The FERP comprises two conditions. During the matching emotion condition, a target face (on the top of the display) and two test faces (bottom left and right) were presented in a triangular arrangement. Participants were instructed to choose one of the two test faces on the bottom that expressed the same emotion (anger, fear, sadness) as the target face on the top of the screen. All three faces were different, and an equal number of male and female faces were presented through the tasks. The target face and the congruent test face always displayed the same facial expression, the incongruent test face always showed neutral expression. Subjects made responses by pressing MR-compatible response buttons in either their left or right thumb in accordance with the position of the chosen probe face. Participants’ decisions were marked by a yellow square on the screen and once it was appeared, subjects were unable to change their response. As mentioned earlier, face photographs were selected from the FACES database43.
Blocks of matching face tasks were interspersed with blocks of a baseline task of matching geometric shapes (matching shape task). During the task participants completed trials involving emotional neutral, abstract geometric forms (circles, vertical and horizontal ellipses) in an analogous configuration like in matching emotion task. Subjects were asked to indicate which shape at the bottom matches the target form at the top. These trials served to maintain attention and allow corticolimbic brain responses to return to the baseline. Similarly, to the previous condition participants response by pressing MR-compatible response buttons in accordance with the position of the chosen test shape. A yellow square on the screen marked the participant’s unchangeable decision.
During the blocks participants’ responses and reaction times (RTs) were recorded.
The whole run consisted of six matching emotion and six matching shape alternating blocks. Each block lasted 30 s, containing six sequential matching trials. Each picture was presented for 5 s, with no interstimulus interval. The whole experiment with a try-out session lasted 370 s. Half of the trials required left-handed, while the other half required right-handed responses within each block.
The experiment started with an instruction period, followed by a try-out session which contained one trial of both conditions (matching emotion and matching shape). After the participant solved the try-out session caption “WAIT” appeared on the screen until the first matching shape block started. Within each trial, the corresponding stimuli sets stayed on the screen for the entire 5 s, even after the participant made a choice. The first matching shape block was followed by the first matching emotion block. Within each matching emotion block, the same target emotional expression was presented for six times. Half of the tasks were presented with male facial expressions, while the other half with female expressions in random order. During the six matching emotions blocks all three target facial expressions were presented twice in alternated order (for more details about the FERP and the experimental design, see Fig. 2).
Schematic design of the Facial Emotion Recognition Paradigm36 and examples of emotional faces from FACES database43 and shapes. (A): The figure shows that the experiment started with a try-out session in which participants solved one matching face and one matching shapes try-out trial. This was followed by task sessions (12 blocks) that were presented successively (first a baseline block which was always followed by a matching emotion block). Each 30 s block contained six matching trials and subjects were instructed to choose one of the two test faces presented at the bottom (left or right) that was identical to the target image (centre top position). Within each trial, the corresponding stimuli sets stayed on the screen for the entire 5 s, even after the participant made a choice. In all emotion block, the same target emotional expression was presented for six times across the trials. (B): Examples of presented emotional faces with four different expressions (neutral, fearful, angry and sad) and geometric shapes (circle, vertical and horizontal ellipses).
All measurements were performed on the same 3 T MRI scanner (MAGNETOM Prismafit Siemens Healthcare, Erlangen, Germany) with a 20-chanel Head/Neck coil.
Functional images were acquired using a 2D single-shot echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence (TR/TE = 2500/30 ms; Flip angle = 76°; 36 axial slices with a thickness of 3 mm; FOV = 192 × 192 mm2; matrix size = 64 × 64; receiver bandwidth = 2170 Hz/ pixel; no gap, interleaved slice order to avoid crosstalk between contiguous slices). For distortion correction purposes, field mapping sequence (TR/TE1/TE2 = 400/4.92/7.38 ms; Flip angle = 60°; 36 axial slices; FOV = 210 × 210 mm2; matrix size = 70 × 70; receiver bandwidth = 290 Hz/pixel) with the same voxel size with 25% gap, orientation and adjustment parameters as the fMRI scan was acquired right after the fMRI measurement.
Anatomical images were obtained using an isotropic T1-weighted 3D-MPRAGE sequence (TR/TI/TE: 2530/1100/3.37 ms; Flip angle = 7°; 176 sagittal slices with thickness of 1 mm; FOV = 256 × 256 mm2; matrix size = 256 × 256; receiver bandwidth = 200 Hz/pixel). The MPRAGE anatomical images were visually checked by MRI experts. There were no brain abnormalities according to the visual inspection of the MR images.
The same preprocessing steps were used in BOLD signal analyses and FC analyses.
Pre-processing steps, BOLD signal and FC analyses were performed using freely available software tools within FMRIB’s Software Library FEAT Version 6.00, part of FSL (FMRIB’s Software Library, FSL, www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl).
Pre-processing procedure included MCFLIRT motion correction, brain extraction46, spatial smoothing (Gaussian kernel, 5 mm full width at half maximum), EPI distortion correction with FSL FUGUE47 and high-pass temporal filtering with 100 s cutoff.
Single-session data sets were registered into the MNI152 standard space using a two-step process. First, the functional image of each subject was registered to that subject’s high-resolution T1 structural scan using BBR (6 degrees-of-freedom)48. Then, each subject’s T1 image was registered to the 2 mm MNI152 standard space using 12 DOF linear fit followed by nonlinear registration (FNIRT, warp resolution = 10 mm)49. Next, for each subject, these two registrations were combined and applied to the first-level statistical maps to take them into standard space.
BOLD analysis was performed to get a comprehensive view of the activation pattern during FERP’s matching emotion condition and extract bilateral amygdala seeds’ timeseries for PPI analysis. The whole brain general linear model (GLM) time-series statistical analysis of individual data sets was performed using FILM (FMRIB’s Improved Linear Model part of FSL).
First level analyses included a single regressor: modelling matching emotion conditions as task active periods and matching shape conditions as baseline periods.
Higher-level mixed-effect analyses were carried out using FLAME1 (FMRIB’s Local Analysis of Mixed Effects) with outlier de-weighting to investigate activation pattern during matching emotion condition.
In order, to make interpretate our findings, mean activation map was calculated (Fig. 3). Statistical map was considered to be significant at Z > 6 and a family-wise error corrected cluster significance threshold of p = 0.0550. According to the activation map, similarly to previous studies36,44, the FERP successfully and robustly engaged the bilateral amygdala and strongly activated brain areas which are involved in emotional face expression recognition. According to the statistical map, we could certainly extract the amygdala seeds’ time-courses during match emotion condition in functional connectivity analysis.
Mean task-dependent activations during the FERP. Image was thresholded using clusters determined by Z > 6 and a corrected cluster significance threshold of p = 0.05. Red–yellow colours indicate brain activation Z-scores during matching emotion tasks. Images are shown in radiological convention.
To examine task related FC, we performed a whole brain PPI analysis in FSL using the pre-processing steps described above.
During the analyses we used left and right amygdala as seed regions.
PPI analysis is a method to finding correlation in neuronal activation between two distant brain structures in a given psychological context (i.e., in matching emotion task). In other words, PPI aims to capture an interaction between the psychological state (the behavioural task) and the functional coupling between two brain areas. In PPI, we principally look for regions which have a higher correlation with the time-course in the seed region in one psychological state (matching emotion tasks) than another (baseline, matching shape tasks). A task-specific increase in the correlation between a seed region and other voxels in the rest of the brain is suggestive of a task-dependent growth in the exchange of information51.
Our seed regions were structurally and functionally constrained.
At first step subcortical and cortical reconstruction and segmentation were implemented on subjects’ T1-weighted anatomical images with usage of Freesurfer 6.0 (https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/fswiki)52. After this procedure, visual quality control was performed by MRI experts. If error was found during reconstruction, error correction was implemented according to the recommended workflow (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/fswiki/RecommendedReconstruction). Anatomical labels of amygdala (both left and right) were extracted for each participant separately based on Freesurfer’s subcortical segmentation atlas53.
Then these T1-weighted seed regions were transformed onto the participant’s functional EPI image through BBR linear registration with 6 DOF using FSL FLIRT (part of the FSL’ s toolbox).
To make sure that our seed regions were significantly active and engaged during the matching emotion task we excluded those voxels which did not show significant activations during the matching emotion task (Z < 2.3).
Finally, we extracted the average time-courses within our subject-specific left and right amygdala seed regions.
After the extraction of subject specific time-courses, PPI analyses were conducted for left and right amygdala seeds separately to define voxels in which activity is more correlated with the time-course in the seed regions in psychological context (i.e., in matching emotion condition), than another. For implementing PPI, during the standard GLM first-level analysis, we used instructions of FSL experts (https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/PPIHowToRun). The design of first-level analysis contained three regressors. The first was the psychological regressor convolved with an HRF which represents task condition and is used to determine condition-specific changes in functional connections between regions. Second was the physiological regressor which represent time-courses taken from seed areas (left and right amygdala). The third variable was the interaction between previous regressors, which identified those voxels that showed greater correlation with the seed region’s time-course in psychological context than in the baseline condition. Time-series statistical analysis was carried out using FILM. In order to explore PIU related functional connections, region of interest (ROI) analyses was performed. During the analyses, FCs between the left and right amygdala and brain regions which are related to EFE recognition were investigated. For the analysis we defined ROIs a priory and extracted the parameter estimation (PE) values of all voxels within the ROIs from each participant’s subject-level PPI contrast, separately for left and right amygdalar seeds. All ROIs were defined according to previous studies, all of which showed to be involved in social cognitive and executive control processes during EFE recognition. These ROIs covered the following areas: left and right superior frontal gyrus (SFG)22, left and right frontal pole (FP)54, left and right posterior and anterior cingulate gyrus (ACG), left and right supramarginal gyrus (SMG)17, left and right middle temporal gyrus (MTG)18. ROIs were defined based on the lateralized Harvard–Oxford Cortical Structural Atlas. Featquery was used to apply ROIs separately to each participant subject-level PPI contrast’s cope image. During analyses, left and right amygdala’s FCs’ PE values were extracted.
Statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS 25.0 (IBM Corp. Released 2017. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 25.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp).
Mean reaction times (RTs) with standard deviations (SD) and medians of the number of errors (ERs) with minimum and maximum values were calculated for each stimulus type in both matching emotion and matching shape conditions. Since none of our questionnaire data showed normal distribution, Spearman’s rank correlation with Benjamini–Hochberg correction (q = 0.05) was used to investigate the associations between the PIUQ total and subscales and the scores of SABAS.
Since none of the FC values showed normal distribution, Spearman’s rank correlations were applied to investigate the associations between PPI and questionnaire scores. To avoid false positives caused by multiple comparisons, Benjamini–Hochberg corrections (q = 0.05) were applied.
The participants’ demographic and questionnaire data are reported in Table 1.
Valid behavioural data were available from all participants. According to the mean RTs and ERs subjects performed the FERP, thus no one was excluded from further analyses based on behavioural outcomes (Table 2). Moreover, no correlation was found between PIUQ scores and behavioural data (RTs and ERs).
As expected, Spearman’s rank correlation analyses showed strong intercorrelation between PIUQ subscales. Furthermore, positive correlations were found between PIUQ subscales and the scores of SABAS (Spearman’s rank correlation results are presented in Table 3).
For the left amygdala based functional connections, no association remained significant after Benjamini–Hochberg corrections (Table 4).
After Benjamini–Hochberg correction, the scores of PIUQ obsession showed positive associations with the FCs between the right amygdala and the left PCG, bilateral ACG, right posterior SMG, bilateral posterior and anterior MTG and bilateral FP. Positive association between PIUQ neglect scores and the FCs between the right amygdala and left ACG and left FP remained significant after Benjamini–Hochberg correction. Only the positive association between PIUQ neglect scores and the FC between right amygdala and right posterior MTG remained significant after Benjamini–Hochberg correction. After corrections, PIUQ total scores correlated positively with the FCs between right amygdala and the bilateral ACG, bilateral PCG, right anterior and posterior MTG, left anterior MTG and bilateral FP (Table 5).
SABAS scores showed positive associations with the FCs between right amygdala and the right posterior SMG, right anterior MTG, left SFG and bilateral FP after Benjamini–Hochberg correction (Table 5).
In the present study we investigated the effect of problematic Internet use and excessive smartphone use (hereafter PIU&ESU) on the functional connections of the bilateral amygdala during the facial emotion recognition task. According to our results, the extent of PIU&ESU showed associations with amygdala’s functional connections in relation with cognitive control functions and social cognition.
The FCs between the right amygdala and frontal regions (frontal poles and superior frontal gyri) positively correlated with questionnaire scores (PIUQ and SABAS). The SFG is mainly involved in the organization of behaviour via cognitive control55. Related to this, the SFG is a core region in emotion regulation processes56, since via its’ orbitofrontal connection regulates the amygdala’s appraisal for emotional stimuli23. FP is a part of the prefrontal cortex (PFC)57 which plays a central role in organising behaviour via its top-down control function during emotion perception16,58,59. Fronto-striatal FCs, especially the connection between the dorsolateral PFC (dlPFC) and amygdala60 are essentials in emotion regulation and recognition61,62. The dlPFC, the ventrolateral PFC and the SFG are parts of the cognitive control network (CCN)22,63,64. The CCN is responsible for the monitoring and representing of contextual information, in service of organizing, controlling, and implementing human behaviour (such as emotion recognition) in harmony with the monitored environmental context63,64. Via cognitive control functions the CCN can regulate amygdala’s sensitivity towards emotional cues in according to the environmental context and behavioural goals16,23,65. Impairments within the CCN go hand in hand with disrupted emotion regulation, which contributes to the development and maintenance of addictive disorders64. Dysfunctional emotion regulation processes previously reported in Internet gaming disorder66, might also appear in PIU67,68 and ESU69. According to these findings, associations between questionnaire scores and the FCs in CCN are crucial in PIU and refer to the impairment of emotion regulation processes.
Our results are also related to social cognitive functions. The FCs between right amygdala and emotion recognition related brain regions (MTG, ACG, PCG and SMG) correlated positively with questionnaire scores (PIUQ, SABAS). SMG plays a key role in the EFE recognition via its’ somatosensory involvement. We understand someone’s emotional state by generating internal somatosensory images of it, to simulate within ourselves how others feel themselves26. Therefore, somatosensory regions, such as the anterior part of SMG, in functional cooperation with the amygdala are indispensable in the recognition of socially relevant information (such as emotion) from faces26,70. The MTG is involved in language processes71, but also related to emotion perception and social cognitive processes such as mentalizing functions18,72,73. Since EFEs provide necessary information about others’ mental state14,15, mentalizing is indispensable in appropriate EFE recognition. The PCG and ACG in functional cooperation with the amygdala contribute to emotional face expression recognition17 and social cognitive evaluation processes74. The PCG is involved in mentalizing75 and in the regulation of attentional processes toward relevant social cues76. The ACG is related to action monitoring in self and others77 and emotional evaluative and regulative mechanisms78,79. Via its functional connection with the dlPFC, ACG also takes part in the attentional control of emotional relevant context17. Alterations within resting-state FCs between ACG and amygdala were previously found in PIU34. Moreover, impaired task-based FCs between these regions were found in cocaine30 and methamphetamine31 users during emotion recognition, which were associated with socially maladaptive behaviours31. Similarly, poorer performance in emotion recognition was previously reported among SNS addicts related to controls32. Furthermore, the performance of emotion recognition in IGD subjects is an independent predictor of the extent of the addiction33. Based on our own and the above cited findings, social cognitive functions, such as mentalisation, but especially the emotional face expression recognition are altered in PIU&ESU.
There are some limitations which must be considered. First, the scores of PIUQ and SABAS showed no correlations with the behavioural performance (RTs, ERs). The most plausible explanation is that the FERP was relatively easy to complete and was not sensitive enough to detect individual differences. It must be noted that the essence of the task was not to detect individual differences rather to get proper amygdala seed time-courses and its’ functional connections for PPI, since the task effectively activates emotion recognition-related cortico-limbic networks, including the amygdala44. Secondly, only five correlations between FCs and PIUQ scores remained significant after Benjamini-Hochberg corrections (Table 4 and Table 5). Therefore, some of our results should be interpreted as less robust evidence and further studies are needed to verify them. Finally, the cross-sectional nature of our investigation limits the possibility of interpretation of what are causes and effects in the relationship between social cognition and PIU&ESU. We do not know if PIU&ESU leads to social cognitive difficulties or vice versa. To understand the nature of the relationship, longitudinal studies are highly needed.
In conclusion, our results released two different functional brain systems that are altered in PIU&ESU. First, the involvement of CCN was revealed. Altered functioning of fronto-striatal FCs are essential in the development and maintenance of addictive disorders64. Decline in cognitive control functions is a common phenomenon among different addictions induced by the continuous compensation of addictive behaviour80,81. Second, the involvement of social cognitive functions (mentalisation and EFE recognition) in PIU&ESU was revealed. The FCs between the amygdala and ACG, PCG, MTG and SMG are essential in intact social cognitive functioning17,18,26. Altered functioning of this connections have been previously described in different substance use disorders27,28,29,30,31 which connected to poorer performance in emotion recognition and socially maladaptive behaviours. These findings help to understand a slice of the altered social cognitive functioning in PIU&ESU and give a well-established starting point for further investigations. During the past decade Internet technology changed our daily life and the way we communicate as well. A significant proportion of social interactions take place in online space, individuals keep in touch more easily via the Internet and time spent with others maybe decreases in the real world. The long-term impact of this extensive social life and communication in online space is not well understood. Therefore, further investigation of social functioning is essential in PIU&ESU. To the best of our knowledge this is the very first study in which social cognitive functions and their neural correlates were investigated in PIU&ESU. Nevertheless, our results emphasized that besides the impairment in brain’s executive and reward systems, the social brain network might be the next candidate to be involved in the pathogenesis of PIU&ESU.
The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Young, K. S. Internet addiction: The emergence of a new clinical disorder. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 1, 237–244 (1998).
Article  Google Scholar 
Saville, B. K., Gisbert, A., Kopp, J. & Telesco, C. Internet addiction and delay discounting in college students. Psychol. Rec. 60, 273–286 (2010).
Article  Google Scholar 
Demetrovics, Z., Szeredi, B. & Rózsa, S. The three-factor model of Internet addiction: The development of the problematic internet use questionnaire. Behav. Res. Methods 40, 563–574 (2008).
Article  Google Scholar 
Park, S., Kang, M. & Kim, E. Social relationship on problematic Internet use (PIU) among adolescents in South Korea: A moderated mediation model of self-esteem and self-control. Comput. Human Behav. 38, 349–357 (2014).
Article  Google Scholar 
Mihajlov, M. & Vejmelka, L. Internet addiction: A review of the first twenty years. Psychiatr. Danub. 29, 260–272 (2017).
Article  Google Scholar 
Akhter, N. Relationship between internet addiction and academic performance among university undergraduates. Educ. Res. Rev. 8, 1793–1796 (2013).
Google Scholar 
Fuchs, M., Riedl, D., Bock, A., Rumpold, G. & Sevecke, K. Pathological internet use—an important comorbidity in child and adolescent psychiatry: Prevalence and correlation patterns in a naturalistic sample of adolescent inpatients. Biomed. Res. Int. https://doi.org/10.1155/2018/1629147 (2018).
Article  Google Scholar 
Altbäcker, A. et al. Problematic internet use is associated with structural alterations in the brain reward system in females. Brain Imaging Behav. 10, 953–959 (2016).
Article  Google Scholar 
Horvath, J. et al. Structural and functional correlates of smartphone addiction. Addict. Behav. 105, 106334 (2020).
Article  Google Scholar 
Zhou, Y. et al. Gray matter abnormalities in internet addiction: A voxel-based morphometry study. Eur. J. Radiol. 79, 92–95 (2011).
Article  Google Scholar 
Darnai, G. et al. Internet addiction and functional brain networks: Task-related fMRI study. Sci. Rep. 9, 1–10 (2019).
Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 
Chen, Z., Poon, K. T. & Cheng, C. Deficits in recognizing disgust facial expressions and Internet addiction: Perceived stress as a mediator. Psychiatry Res. 254, 211–217 (2017).
Article  Google Scholar 
Beer, J. S. & Ochsner, K. N. Social cognition: A multi level analysis. Brain Res. 1079, 98–105 (2006).
Article  CAS  Google Scholar 
van Kleef, G. A. How emotions regulate social life. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 18, 184–188 (2009).
Article  Google Scholar 
Brackett, M. A., Rivers, S. E., Shiffman, S., Lerner, N. & Salovey, P. Relating emotional abilities to social functioning: A comparison of self-report and performance measures of emotional intelligence. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 91, 780–795 (2006).
Article  Google Scholar 
Phillips, M. L., Drevets, W. C., Rauch, S. L. & Lane, R. Neurobiology of emotion perception I: The neural basis of normal emotion perception. Biol. Psychiatry 54, 504–514 (2003).
Article  Google Scholar 
Diano, M. et al. Dynamic changes in amygdala psychophysiological connectivity reveal distinct neural networks for facial expressions of basic emotions. Sci. Rep. 7, 1–13 (2017).
Article  ADS  Google Scholar 
Allison, T., Puce, A. & McCarthy, G. Allison et al., 2000. Trends Cogn Sci 4, 267–278 (2000).
Gur, R. C. et al. Brain activation during facial emotion processing. Neuroimage 16, 651–662 (2002).
Article  Google Scholar 
Sabatinelli, D. et al. Emotional perception: Meta-analyses of face and natural scene processing. Neuroimage 54, 2524–2533 (2011).
Article  Google Scholar 
Habel, U. et al. Amygdala activation and facial expressions: Explicit emotion discrimination versus implicit emotion processing. Neuropsychologia 45, 2369–2377 (2007).
Article  Google Scholar 
Frank, D. W. et al. Emotion regulation: Quantitative meta-analysis of functional activation and deactivation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 45, 202–211 (2014).
Article  CAS  Google Scholar 
Ochsner, K. N. & Gross, J. J. The cognitive control of emotion. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 242–249 (2005).
Article  Google Scholar 
Koob, G. F. & le Moal, M. Addiction and the brain antireward system. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59, 29–53 (2007).
Article  Google Scholar 
Sladky, R. et al. Disrupted effective connectivity between the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex in social anxiety disorder during emotion discrimination revealed by dynamic causal modeling for fMRI. Cereb. Cortex 25, 895–903 (2015).
Article  Google Scholar 
Adolphs, R., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., Cooper, G. & Damasio, A. R. A role for somatosensory cortices in the visual recognition of emotion as revealed by three-dimensional lesion mapping. J. Neurosci. 20, 2683–2690 (2000).
Article  CAS  Google Scholar 
le Berre, A. P. Emotional processing and social cognition in alcohol use disorder. Neuropsychology 33, 808–821 (2019).
Article  Google Scholar 
Foisy, M. L. et al. Impaired emotional facial expression recognition in alcoholics: Are these deficits specific to emotional cues?. Psychiatry Res. 150, 33–41 (2007).
Article  Google Scholar 
le Berre, A. P. et al. Chronic alcohol consumption and its effect on nodes of frontocerebellar and limbic circuitry: Comparison of effects in France and the United States. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 4635–4653 (2014).
Article  Google Scholar 
Crunelle, C. L. et al. Dysfunctional amygdala activation and connectivity with the prefrontal cortex in current cocaine users. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 4222–4230 (2015).
Article  Google Scholar 
Payer, D. E. et al. Differences in cortical activity between methamphetamine-dependent and healthy individuals performing a facial affect matching task. Drug Alcohol Depend. 93, 93–102 (2008).
Article  CAS  Google Scholar 
Ünal-Aydın, P., Balıkçı, K., Sönmez, İ & Aydın, O. Associations between emotion recognition and social networking site addiction. Psychiatry Res. 284, 112673 (2020).
Article  Google Scholar 
Aydın, O., Güçlü, M., Ünal-Aydın, P. & Spada, M. M. Metacognitions and emotion recognition in Internet Gaming Disorder among adolescents. Addict. Behav. Rep. 12, 100296 (2020).
Google Scholar 
Cheng, H. & Liu, J. Alterations in amygdala connectivity in internet addiction disorder. Sci. Rep. 10, 1–10 (2020).
Google Scholar 
Chun, J. W. et al. Altered brain activity and the effect of personality traits in excessive smartphone use during facial emotion processing. Sci. Rep. 7, 1–13 (2017).
Article  Google Scholar 
Hariri, A. R. et al. Serotonin transporter genetic variation and the response of the human amygdala. Science 1979(297), 400–403 (2002).
Article  ADS  Google Scholar 
Oldfield, R. C. Hand preference. Encycl. Clin. Neurosci. 9, 97–113 (1971).
CAS  Google Scholar 
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J. & Erbaugh, J. An inventory for measuring depression the difficulties inherent in obtaining. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004 (1961).
Article  Google Scholar 
Poli, R. Internet addiction update: Diagnostic criteria, assessment and prevalence. Neuropsychiatry 07, 4–8 (2017).
Article  Google Scholar 
Koronczai, B. et al. Confirmation of the three-factor model of problematic internet use on off-line adolescent and adult samples. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 14, 657–664 (2011).
Article  Google Scholar 
Csibi, S., Demetrovics, Z. & Szabó, A. Development and psychometric validation of the brief smartphone addiction scale (BSAS) with schoolchidren. Psychiatr. Hung. 31, 71–77 (2016).
Google Scholar 
Griffiths, M. A ‘components’ model of addiction within a biopsychosocial framework. J. Subst. Use 10, 191–197 (2005).
Article  Google Scholar 
Ebner, N. C., Riediger, M. & Lindenberger, U. FACES-a database of facial expressions in young, middle-aged, and older women and men: Development and validation. Behav. Res. Methods 42, 351–362 (2010).
Article  Google Scholar 
Kirsch, P. et al. Oxytocin modulates neural circuitry for social cognition and fear in humans. J. Neurosci. 25, 11489–11493 (2005).
Article  CAS  Google Scholar 
Nagy, S. A. et al. Childhood maltreatment results in altered deactivation of reward processing circuits in depressed patients: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of a facial emotion recognition task. Neurobiol. Stress 15, 100399 (2021).
Article  Google Scholar 
Smith, S. M. Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum. Brain. Mapp. 17, 143–155 (2002).
Article  Google Scholar 
Jenkinson, M. Improving the registration of B0-disorted Epi images using calculated cost function weights: We 202. Neuroimage 22, (2004).
Jenkinson, M., Bannister, P., Brady, M. & Smith, S. Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. Neuroimage 17, 825–841 (2002).
Article  Google Scholar 
Andersson, J., Smith, S. & Jenkinson, M. FNIRT-FMRIB’s non-linear image registration tool. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2008 (2008).
Worsley, K. J. Statistical analysis of activation images. Funct. MRI Introd. Methods 14, 251–270 (2001).
Google Scholar 
O’Reilly, J. X., Woolrich, M. W., Behrens, T. E. J., Smith, S. M. & Johansen-Berg, H. Tools of the trade: Psychophysiological interactions and functional connectivity. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7, 604–609 (2012).
Article  Google Scholar 
Dale, A. M., Fischl, B. & Sereno, M. I. Cortical surface-based analysis. Neuroimage 9, 179–194 (1999).
Article  CAS  Google Scholar 
Fischl, B. et al. Whole brain segmentation. Neuron 33, 341–355 (2002).
Article  CAS  Google Scholar 
Koch, S. B. J., Mars, R. B., Toni, I. & Roelofs, K. Emotional control, reappraised. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 95, 528–534 (2018).
Article  Google Scholar 
Egner, T. & Hirsch, J. The neural correlates and functional integration of cognitive control in a stroop task. Neuroimage 24, 539–547 (2005).
Article  Google Scholar 
Frank, D. W. et al. Emotion regulation: Quantitative meta-analysis of functional activation and deactivation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 45, 202–211 (2014).
Article  CAS  Google Scholar 
Koechlin, E. Frontal pole function: What is specifically human?. Trends Cogn Sci 15, 241 (2011).
Article  Google Scholar 
Koechlin, E., Ody, C. & Kouneiher, F. The architecture of cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex. Science 1979(302), 1181–1185 (2003).
Article  ADS  Google Scholar 
Miller, E. K. & Miller, E. K. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2000.pdf. Nature Reviews/Neuroscience 1, 13–15 (2000).
Google Scholar 
Bramson, B. et al. Human lateral frontal pole contributes to control over emotional approach–avoidance actions. J. Neurosci. 40, 2925–2934 (2020).
Article  CAS  Google Scholar 
Banks, S. J., Eddy, K. T., Angstadt, M., Nathan, P. J. & Luan Phan, K. Amygdala-frontal connectivity during emotion regulation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2, 303–312 (2007).
Article  Google Scholar 
Motzkin, J. C., Philippi, C. L., Wolf, R. C., Baskaya, M. K. & Koenigs, M. Ventromedial prefrontal cortex is critical for the regulation of amygdala activity in humans. Biol. Psychiatry 77, 276–284 (2015).
Article  Google Scholar 
Ridderinkhof, K. R., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., Segalowitz, S. J. & Carter, C. S. Neurocognitive mechanisms of cognitive control: The role of prefrontal cortex in action selection, response inhibition, performance monitoring, and reward-based learning. Brain Cogn. 56, 129–140 (2004).
Article  Google Scholar 
Wilcox, C. E., Pommy, J. M. & Adinoff, B. Neural circuitry of impaired emotion regulation in substance use disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 173, 344–361 (2016).
Article  Google Scholar 
Campos, J. J., Frankel, C. B. & Camras, L. On the nature of emotion regulation. Child Dev. 75(377), 394 (2004).
Google Scholar 
Wu, L. L. et al. A role for the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in enhancing regulation of both craving and negative emotions in internet gaming disorder: A randomized trial. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 36, 29–37 (2020).
Article  CAS  Google Scholar 
Brand, M., Young, K. S., Laier, C., Wölfling, K. & Potenza, M. N. Integrating psychological and neurobiological considerations regarding the development and maintenance of specific internet-use disorders: An interaction of person-affect-cognition-execution (I-PACE) model. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 71, 252–266 (2016).
Article  Google Scholar 
Casale, S., Caplan, S. E. & Fioravanti, G. Positive metacognitions about Internet use: The mediating role in the relationship between emotional dysregulation and problematic use. Addict. Behav. 59, 84–88 (2016).
Article  Google Scholar 
Yildiz, M. Emotion regulation strategies as predictors of internet addiction and smartphone addiction in adolescents. J. Educ. Sci. Psychol. 7(66), 78 (2017).
Google Scholar 
Adolphs, R. & Spezio, M. Chapter 20 Role of the amygdala in processing visual social stimuli. Prog. Brain Res. 156, 363–378 (2006).
Article  Google Scholar 
McDermott, K. B., Petersen, S. E., Watson, J. M. & Ojemann, J. G. A procedure for identifying regions preferentially activated by attention to semantic and phonological relations using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuropsychologia 41, 293–303 (2003).
Article  Google Scholar 
Xu, J. et al. Delineating functional segregations of the human middle temporal gyrus with resting-state functional connectivity and coactivation patterns. Hum. Brain Mapp. 40, 5159–5171 (2019).
Article  Google Scholar 
Schurz, M., Tholen, M. G., Perner, J., Mars, R. B. & Sallet, J. Specifying the brain anatomy underlying temporo-parietal junction activations for theory of mind: A review using probabilistic atlases from different imaging modalities. Hum. Brain Mapp. 38, 4788–4805 (2017).
Article  Google Scholar 
Li, W., Mai, X. & Liu, C. The default mode network and social understanding of others: What do brain connectivity studies tell us. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8, 1–15 (2014).
Article  Google Scholar 
Mars, R. B. et al. On the relationship between the ‘default mode network’ and the ‘social brain’. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6, 1–9 (2012).
Article  Google Scholar 
Leech, R., Kamourieh, S., Beckmann, C. F. & Sharp, D. J. Fractionating the default mode network: Distinct contributions of the ventral and dorsal posterior cingulate cortex to cognitive control. J. Neurosci. 31, 3217–3224 (2011).
Article  CAS  Google Scholar 
Amodio, D. M. & Frith, C. D. Meeting of minds: The medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 268–277 (2006).
Article  CAS  Google Scholar 
Etkin, A., Egner, T. & Kalisch, R. Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 85–93 (2011).
Article  Google Scholar 
Etkin, A., Egner, T., Peraza, D. M., Kandel, E. R. & Hirsch, J. Resolving emotional conflict: A role for the rostral anterior cingulate cortex in modulating activity in the Amygdala. Neuron 51, 871–882 (2006).
Article  CAS  Google Scholar 
Tiffany, S. T. Cognitive concepts of craving. Alcohol Res. Health 23, 215–224 (1999).
CAS  Google Scholar 
Brand, M. et al. The interaction of person-affect-cognition-execution (I-PACE) model for addictive behaviors: Update, generalization to addictive behaviors beyond internet-use disorders, and specification of the process character of addictive behaviors. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 104, 1–10 (2019).
Article  Google Scholar 
Download references
This study was supported by the Hungarian Brain Research Program 20017-1.2.1-NKP−2017-00002 government-based fund. Our research was partly financed by the Higher Education Institutional Excellence Program of the Ministry of Human Capacities in Hungary, within the framework of the 5th thematic program of the University of Pécs, Hungary (20765/3/2018/FEKUSTRAT). G.P. was supported by the János Bolyai Research Scholarship of the Hungarian Academy of Sciences. S.A.N. was supported by the János Bolyai Research Scholarship of the Hungarian Academy of Sciences, ÚNKP-20-5-PTE-715 New National Excellence Program of the Ministry for Innovation and Technology, and PTE ÁOK-KA-2020-08. A.T.S. was supported by the ÚNKP-21-3-I-PTE-1001 New National Excellence Program of the Ministry for Innovation and Technology.
Department of Neurology, Medical School, University of Pécs, Rét Street 2, 7623, Pecs, Hungary
Ákos Arató, Gábor Perlaki, Anna Tímea Szente, Gréta Kis-Jakab, Eszter Áfra, Husamalddin Ali Alhour, Norbert Kovács, József Janszky & Gergely Darnai
ELKH-PTE Clinical Neuroscience MR Research Group, Pecs, Hungary
Szilvia Anett Nagy, Gábor Perlaki, Gergely Orsi, Norbert Kovács, József Janszky & Gergely Darnai
Pécs Diagnostic Centre, Pecs, Hungary
Szilvia Anett Nagy, Gábor Perlaki, Gergely Orsi & Gréta Kis-Jakab
Department of Neurosurgery, Medical School, University of Pécs, Pecs, Hungary
Szilvia Anett Nagy & Gergely Orsi
Neurobiology of Stress Research Group, Szentágothai Research Centre, University of Pécs, Pecs, Hungary
Szilvia Anett Nagy
Department of Laboratory Medicine, Medical School, University of Pécs, Pecs, Hungary
Szilvia Anett Nagy
Department of Behavioural Sciences, Medical School, University of Pécs, Pecs, Hungary
Eszter Áfra & Gergely Darnai
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
Á.A. was responsible for writing the manuscript and supervising the whole process. S.A.N., G.P. and G.O. were responsible for M.R.I. measurements and the statistical analysis. D.G., A.T.S., G.K.-J., E.Á. and H.A.A. were responsible for the manuscript review and critique. D.G., N.K., and J.J. contributed equally to the conception, organization, and execution of the research project.
Correspondence to József Janszky.
The authors declare no competing interests.
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
Reprints and Permissions
Arató, Á., Nagy, S.A., Perlaki, G. et al. Emotional face expression recognition in problematic Internet use and excessive smartphone use: task-based fMRI study. Sci Rep 13, 354 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-27172-0
Download citation
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-27172-0
Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:
Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.
Advertisement
Scientific Reports (Sci Rep) ISSN 2045-2322 (online)
© 2023 Springer Nature Limited
Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

source

Leave a Reply

This site uses Akismet to reduce spam. Learn how your comment data is processed.